Speaking to babies in a sing-song voice could help them learn languages, experts say

Speaking to babies in sing-song speech could help them learn the language, experts from the University of Cambridge have found

This article contains affiliate links. We may earn a small commission on items purchased through this article, but that does not affect our editorial judgement.

Speak to babies in a sing-song voice, Cambridge experts say. Picture: GettySpeak to babies in a sing-song voice, Cambridge experts say. Picture: Getty
Speak to babies in a sing-song voice, Cambridge experts say. Picture: Getty

Speaking to babies in sing-song speech could help them learn the language, experts from the University of Cambridge found, saying infants learn languages from rhythmic information – the rise and fall of tone – as seen in nursery rhymes or songs. They also discovered babies do not begin to process phonetic information – the smallest sounds of speech – until they are around seven months old.

The researchers said the findings, published in the journal Nature Communications, challenge the view that phonetic information – typically represented by the alphabet – is the key to language learning and suggests dyslexia and developmental language disorder may be associated with rhythm perception rather than difficulties with processing phonetic information. Study author Professor Usha Goswami, a neuroscientist at the University of Cambridge, said: “Our research shows that the individual sounds of speech are not processed reliably until around seven months, even though most infants can recognise familiar words like ‘bottle’ by this point.

Hide Ad
Hide Ad

"From then individual speech sounds are still added in very slowly – too slowly to form the basis of language. We believe that speech rhythm information is the hidden glue underpinning the development of a well-functioning language system. Parents should talk and sing to their babies as much as possible or use infant-directed speech like nursery rhymes because it will make a difference to language outcome.”

Previously, it was thought infants learn small sound elements and add them together to make words but to understand whether that was the case the researchers recorded the brain activity of 50 infants at four, seven and 11 months old as they watched a video of a primary school teacher singing 18 nursery rhymes.

The team used special algorithms to interpret how the infants were encoding this information in the brain and found that phonetic encoding in babies emerged gradually over the first year of life, beginning with dental sounds (produced by the upper front teeth) – such as “d” for “daddy” – and nasal sounds (produced when airflow is directed through the nose) – such as “m” for “mummy”.

Prof Goswami said: “Infants can use rhythmic information like a scaffold or skeleton to add phonetic information on to. For example, they might learn that the rhythm pattern of English words is typically strong-weak, as in ‘daddy’ or ‘mummy’, with the stress on the first syllable. They can use this rhythm pattern to guess where one word ends and another begins when listening to natural speech.”

Hide Ad
Hide Ad

She said rhythm is a universal aspect of every language where all babies “are exposed to… a strong beat structure with a strong syllable twice a second”. Prof Goswami added: “We’re biologically programmed to emphasise this when speaking to babies.”

The study forms part of the BabyRhythm project led by Prof Goswami, which is investigating how language is related to dyslexia and developmental language disorder. She said there is a long history of trying to explain these in terms of phonetic problems but the evidence does not add up, and individual differences in children’s language learning skills may originate with rhythm.

Related topics:

Comment Guidelines

National World encourages reader discussion on our stories. User feedback, insights and back-and-forth exchanges add a rich layer of context to reporting. Please review our Community Guidelines before commenting.